A dark grey Canadian sphynx cat looks directly at the camera with green eyes and his ears turned to point forward. The image is surrounded by a yellow border and overlaid with the headline, "Minds of their own: Some animals have complex emotions— much like us"
Ed, a Canadian sphynx cat, is curious, outgoing, affectionate, and highly responsive to human emotion. He’s also talkative: Say his name, he purrs. In this portrait, his forward-tilted ears show he’s alert and his narrowed pupils that he’s relaxed.
Photograph by VINCENT LAGRANGE

How these two photographers got inside the minds of animals

Jasper Doest and Paolo Verzone were among the photographers who captured the rich inner lives of animals for National Geographic’s October cover story.

ByEmily Martin and Sylvia Mphofe
September 15, 2022
6 min read

We know that dogs and cats can feel happy, stressed, grumpy, or scared—and dogs can even “catch” their owners’ emotions. But what about rats that show empathy or monkeys with a strong sense of self? The team behind National Geographic’s October feature story explores how a wide array of animals exhibit complex emotions similar to ours.

In addition to Vincent Lagrange’s cover image of a Canadian sphynx cat, National Geographic recruited several talented photographers to capture animal minds across the globe. We spoke to two of them, Jasper Doest and Paolo Verzone, for more about how they were able to photograph the “animal minds” of their subjects.

What’s the story behind the cover?

This month’s cover story explores the many ways animals are capable of displaying sophisticated emotions, such as empathy and kindness. Behavioral studies have given us a deeper understanding that animals possess not only cognitive abilities but emotional ones too.

A Japanese Macaque holds a small dirty mirror in front of their face looking deep into their reflection as one hand gently touches the edge of the mirror's frame.
A Japanese macaque stares at its reflection in a moped mirror. Some monkeys appear to recognize the image that they’re seeing as themselves, as do apes. Scientists use the so-called mirror test to determine if animals exhibit self-awareness. In humans this attribute develops around 18 months or later. 
Photograph by JASPER DOEST

Doest photographed the Japanese macaque staring at its reflection in the mirror, as well as the Australian shepherd getting scanned in a magnetic resonance imaging machine. He says both images presented different challenges and called for him to think on his feet. 

Photographing the Australian shepherd was technically difficult because Doest couldn’t use lighting in the sterile white room where the image was taken. Doing so would potentially scare the dog and also ruin the MRI output. 

“I found a way to keep my distance but still get up close and personal, so people feel like they’re still there with the dog,” he says.

An Australian shepherd dog lies still in a large magnetic resonance machine
Knopfi, an Australian shepherd being studied at the University of Vienna, learned how to lie motionless in a magnetic resonance imaging machine. Observing dog brains, scientists have found activity in areas similar to those in humans. Words of praise lit up a dog’s reward centers. Videos of caregivers activated regions tied to attachment.
Photograph by JASPER DOEST

For the Japanese macaque, Doest photographed the animal in a park where wild monkeys come and go as they please. Many distractions surrounded him, he says, which forced him to be patient and wait for that right opportunity, instead of running around with the monkeys. 

Although each worked on their own, Doest and Verzone share similar sentiments when it comes to the impact of this story.

Doest hopes readers will look at animals differently and gain a new sense of empathy for their experience. “We’re not that different,” he says, of humans and animals. “We look different and maybe behave differently, but it doesn't mean we’re complete opposites or are more important than they are.”  

Verzone agrees that animals can teach us so much if we pay attention to them. He was tasked with photographing a behavioral study at Tel Aviv University that studied consolation behavior in rats—specifically, if they would free a fellow rat trapped in a tube. Researchers found that the rats showed compassion for the trapped rat and freed it, but typically only if it belonged to their social group.

“I learned so much from photographing them,” Verzone says. “Their capabilities to empathize surprised me completely.” 

The photo shoot came with its own set of challenges—including the smell of rat excrement in the lab—but Verzone approached it with an open mind. He allowed himself an extra hour to observe the rats and listen to the scientists discuss their work. 

Two brown and white rats sniff for each other while one rat is contained in a clear plastic cylinder with air holes.
“Rats,” says Inbal Ben-Ami Bartal, a neuroscientist at Tel Aviv University, “show the basic components of empathy.” In a study, she tested the rodents to see whether they would free another rat trapped in a tube. She discovered that they help only those belonging to their own social group. Adolescent rats, though, don’t discriminate.
Photograph by PAOLO VERZONE

“This was not in my field of work. I rarely interact with animals, so it was an interesting challenge for me,” Verzone, a portrait photographer, explains. “I immediately went into this mindset of portraits … I had to understand them.”

In setting up the perfect image, Verzone also played around with the lighting, asking the scientists to let him know if the rats were showing signs of discomfort, until he felt a connection with his subject.

By the end of the shoot, the Italian photographer gained a life lesson: Empathy can come naturally for people, but others can develop their own emotional capacities by learning more about the inner lives of animals.

“Learning from them is an important thing for us,” he says. “For a better dialogue with nature, this story is the key to the future.”